search
Back to results

Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Reward Learning in Subclinical Depression.

Primary Purpose

Dysphoria

Status
Unknown status
Phase
Not Applicable
Locations
United Kingdom
Study Type
Interventional
Intervention
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)
Sponsored by
University of Oxford
About
Eligibility
Locations
Arms
Outcomes
Full info

About this trial

This is an interventional other trial for Dysphoria focused on measuring Transcranial direct current stimulation, Biological Psychiatry, Depression, Decision-making

Eligibility Criteria

18 Years - 45 Years (Adult)All SexesAccepts Healthy Volunteers

Inclusion Criteria:

  • Participant is willing and able to give informed consent for participation in the study
  • Participant has a score of >9 on Beck's Depression Inventory II (BDI-II)
  • Fluent English-speaking
  • Right-handed

Exclusion Criteria:

  • Currently taking psychoactive medications
  • Personal or family history of epileptic fits or seizures
  • Family history of extreme mood fluctuations or bipolar disorder
  • Currently pregnant or current likelihood of becoming pregnant
  • Significant suicidal ideation or depression requiring immediate clinical referral

Sites / Locations

  • FMRIB Centre, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of OxfordRecruiting

Arms of the Study

Arm 1

Arm 2

Arm Type

Experimental

Sham Comparator

Arm Label

Bifrontal tDCS

Sham tDCS

Arm Description

20 minutes of 2 mA transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), with the anode placed over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cathode placed over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (F3 and F4 according to the 10/20 international EEG system, respectively). The stimulation will be applied before or during task performance, depending on the condition assignment.

Participants receive 20 minutes of sham tDCS, with the anode placed over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cathode placed over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (F3 and F4 according to the 10/20 international EEG system, respectively). In sham tDCS, stimulation starts with 8s fade in followed by 30s direct current followed by 5s fade out followed by 870s without any stimulation. The stimulation will be applied before or during task performance, depending on the condition assignment.

Outcomes

Primary Outcome Measures

Change in learning rate
In our previous study (Overman et al., 2021), model comparison showed that participants' behaviour on this task was best fit by a computational model combining: a Rescorla-Wagner learning rule with a Softmax function, including two separate learning rates for wins and losses; an inverse temperature parameter accounting for choice randomness; and a tendency parameter capturing a potential tendency to favour one shape over the other. Since the current study aims to replicate our previous findings, we will use the same model. The key hypothesis-driven variable of interest for analysis is the win learning rate.
Change in proportion of win-driven choices
In addition to the computational model, we will also use a non-computational measure, the percentage of "win-driven choices". This is calculated from trials in which the win and loss are both associated with the same shape ("neutral" trials). What shape the participant chooses on the next trial will depend on whether s/he is more influenced by the current win or loss outcome. If the win outcome has a greater influence, the participant will choose the same shape again on the next trial. If the loss is more influential, the participant will avoid the current shape and instead choose the other shape. The proportion of "win-driven choices" is the proportion of trials in which participants choose the same shape on trial n+1 that was associated with both a win and a loss outcome on trial n. The key prediction is that this will be increased by online tDCS.

Secondary Outcome Measures

Change in ability to adjust learning rate to volatility
As an exploratory analysis, we will test whether bifrontal tDCS might improve participants' ability to adjust their learning rates to the volatility context. The two outcome measures will be the difference in learning rates between stable and volatile blocks, e.g. the difference in win and/or loss learning rates contrasted between the wins-volatile and losses-volatile blocks (i.e. Win delta LR = win learning rate in wins-volatile blocks minus win learning rate in losses-volatile blocks; Loss delta LR = loss learning rate in losses volatile blocks minus loss learning rate in wins volatile blocks).

Full Information

First Posted
December 22, 2017
Last Updated
April 13, 2021
Sponsor
University of Oxford
Collaborators
Medical Research Council
search

1. Study Identification

Unique Protocol Identification Number
NCT03393312
Brief Title
Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Reward Learning in Subclinical Depression.
Official Title
Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Reward Learning in Subclinical Depression.
Study Type
Interventional

2. Study Status

Record Verification Date
April 2021
Overall Recruitment Status
Unknown status
Study Start Date
February 2, 2018 (Actual)
Primary Completion Date
December 1, 2021 (Anticipated)
Study Completion Date
December 1, 2021 (Anticipated)

3. Sponsor/Collaborators

Responsible Party, by Official Title
Principal Investigator
Name of the Sponsor
University of Oxford
Collaborators
Medical Research Council

4. Oversight

Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Drug Product
No
Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Device Product
No
Data Monitoring Committee
No

5. Study Description

Brief Summary
This project will test whether transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) can alter reward learning behaviour in subclinical depression. tDCS is a neuromodulation technique that uses weak electrical current to increase (anodal stimulation) or decrease (cathodal stimulation) the excitability of the stimulated brain region. A growing body of evidence indicates that repeated administration of prefrontal tDCS can ameliorate symptoms of depression. A main characteristic of depression is that patients show a bias towards processing negative relative to positive information. Previously, we have found that a single session of prefrontal tDCS applied during task performance increased learning rate for reward outcomes in healthy adults. Here, we will test whether stimulation induces a similar behavioural effect in individuals with subclinical depression. We will test the prediction that tDCS will increase learning rates for reward outcomes in a reinforcement learning task. The findings will contribute to understanding the cognitive effects of prefrontal tDCS in subclinical depression. The ultimate aim, to be explored through further studies, is to understand and improve how tDCS might be used in the treatment of depressive disorders.
Detailed Description
The development of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques offers new approaches for the treatment of depression. TMS is an approved treatment for treatment-resistant depression in the UK and USA. tDCS is currently under investigation as a potentially cheaper, safer and more accessible alternative. A recent meta-analysis suggests that it has a moderate antidepressant effect (Razza et al., 2020). One potential way of improving the antidepressant efficacy of tDCS might be to combine it with a reward learning task. There is evidence from rodent and human studies that tDCS enhances activity-dependent synaptic plasticity and behavioural learning and retention (Fritsch et al., 2010; O'Shea et al., 2017; Reis et al., 2009). In depression, people prioritize the processing of negative information at the cost of positive information and this negative bias is theorized to play a major role in the maintenance of depressive symptoms (Kube, Schwarting, Rozenkrantz, Glombiewski, & Rief, 2020). Therefore, by having depressed participants learn from reward, and increase their learning/retention via tDCS, this could potentially counteract negative bias and thus increase the antidepressant potential of tDCS. In an earlier proof-of-concept study in young healthy volunteers (without low mood), we showed that tDCS applied during, but not before, an information-bias learning task increased reward learning rates. Here, we will test for the same effect in young healthy volunteers with subclinical depression. The study will involve online screening, a face-to-face screening session and two tDCS + learning sessions. Task performance will be fitted with a computational reinforcement learning model. Analyses will include both computational and non-computational measures of task behaviour. Primary hypotheses: Bifrontal compared to sham tDCS applied during the information-bias learning task will increase the learning rate from positive outcomes. Bifrontal compared to sham tDCS applied during the task will increase the percentage of win-driven choices. The effects of bifrontal tDCS will be cognitive state dependent, i.e. the predicted effects will be specific to stimulation applied during but not prior to learning. Secondary hypothesis: A growing body of evidence suggests that depression and anxiety are characterised by a reduced ability to adjust learning rates to volatility of action-outcome contingencies (Browning, Behrens, Jocham, O'Reilly, & Bishop, 2015; Gagne, Zika, Dayan, & Bishop, 2020; Pulcu & Browning, 2017). Therefore, we will also investigate whether bifrontal tDCS might increase participants' ability to adjust their learning rates to the volatility context. This is exploratory, since there is no current evidence that tDCS can improve this ability. Sample size: Our primary goal is to test hypothesis 1 that bifrontal compared to sham tDCS applied during the information-bias learning task will increase the learning rate from positive outcomes. In our previous study in healthy volunteers we observed this effect, which was most pronounced in the losses-volatile condition (paired t-test contrasting bifrontal and sham tDCS in the losses volatile blocks: t(19) = 2.88, p = .009), with an effect size of Cohen's d = 0.522. To detect an effect of this size with a power of 80% and an alpha level of .05 requires 31 participants. To account for a potential overestimation of the effect size we will recruit 40 participants, which should yield a power level of .89 for the effect size observed in our previous study. To keep sample sizes equal across conditions, we will recruit another 40 participants for the offline condition. Screening: Participants will be asked to fill out a pre-screening online depression questionnaire (Beck Depression Inventory - II (BDI)) and a safety screening form to identify potential contraindications to tDCS. Participants with a BDI score of ≥10 will be invited to a 1-hour screening session. The structured clinical interview (SCID) will be administered and the researcher will interview the participant about their tDCS safety screening questionnaire. If there are no contraindications to tDCS and no indications of current or history of bipolar disorder, the researcher will schedule two tDCS testing sessions. Experimental task: Participants will perform the Information Bias Learning Task developed by (Pulcu & Browning, 2017). The same task protocol as in our previous study will be used (Overman, Sarrazin, Browning, & O'Shea, 2021). In short, participants will be asked to press a button to choose between two shapes on each trial. After they choose, the word "win" and "loss" will appear on the screen, each associated with one of the two shapes. The wins and losses are independent of each other, i.e. both can also appear with the same shape. If the chosen shape is associated with a win this leads to a 10 pence financial gain; a loss is minus 10 pence. The cumulative total is shown continuously in the bottom centre of the screen throughout the task. Participants start with a total of £1.50. This task is designed to assess the extent to which participants choices are relatively influenced by win vs. loss outcomes. Participants will perform 6 task blocks of 80 trials. In the first and sixth block ("both volatile blocks"), the wins and losses each have a 75% probability of being associated with one of the shapes. The shape the wins or losses are associated with changes over time. In the second to fifth task blocks, one outcome is associated with one shape in 75% of the trials. The shape the wins or losses are associated with changes over time. The other outcome is associated with both shapes in 50% of the trials. Each participant performs two "wins-volatile" and two "losses-volatile" blocks in alternating order. Half of the participants perform a "win-volatile" block first, and the other half a "loss-volatile" block first. Testing sessions: Participants will be asked to fill out mood and anxiety questionnaires. At the beginning of the first session the Trait subscale of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)(Spielberger, 1983) will be completed. In both sessions, before and after performing the task participants will complete the State subscale of the STAI and the Positive and Negative Affect Scales (PANAS)(Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988)). The researcher will explain the computerised task, and the participant will get the opportunity to practise the task. The researcher will then set up the tDCS equipment with the anode over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the cathode over the right DLPFC, in the F3 and F4 EEG electrode positions. The researcher will briefly test the tDCS to ensure that the participant is comfortable with the stimulation. The participant will perform one ("both volatile") baseline block of the task without tDCS. In the 'online' condition, tDCS will be applied during the task - for a duration of 20 minutes at 2 mA during the second and third task blocks. After the stimulation ends, participants will perform the remaining task blocks 4 and 5 (to test for persistence of any effects post-tDCS), followed by a repeat of the "both volatile" block performed at baseline. In the 'offline' condition, all procedures will be the same except that stimulation will be applied after the baseline ("both volatile") block and before the remaining blocks, while participants sit at rest. Statistical analysis: To test hypothesis 1, that bifrontal compared to sham tDCS applied during the task will increase the learning rate from win outcomes, we will run a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the learning rates in the "wins-volatile" and "losses-volatile" blocks, including the within-subject factors tDCS Condition (bifrontal vs. sham), Valence (win vs. loss outcomes), Volatility (wins-volatile vs. losses-volatile blocks) and Time (first vs. second half of the four task blocks). Baseline learning rates for wins and losses in the first task block of the first session will be included as covariates. First, we will test for an interaction effect between tDCS Condition and Valence. To test our key prediction, we will run a priori planned contrasts of real vs. sham tDCS on win learning rates. Since in our previous study, the tDCS-induced increase in win learning rates was most pronounced in the losses-volatile condition, we will contrast the real-sham win learning rate separately for the losses-volatile and wins-volatile conditions. To test hypothesis 2 that bifrontal compared to sham tDCS will increase the proportion of win-driven choices, we will run an ANOVA on the proportion of win-driven choices including the factors tDCS Condition, Volatility and Time. We will test for a main effect of tDCS Condition, as well as for an interaction effect between tDCS Condition and Volatility. The interaction effect will be followed up by planned contrasts of bifrontal versus sham tDCS, separately for the wins-volatile condition and the losses-volatile conditions. To compare the effect of online versus offline bifrontal tDCS on the win learning rate and the proportion of win-driven choices (hypothesis 3), we will run ANOVAs on the combined dataset (online and offline condition), adding the factor "Stimulation Time" (before vs. during task performance). For learning rates, we will test for a main effect of Stimulation Time and/or an interaction between Stimulation Time and tDCS Condition and/or Valence. Follow-up ANOVAs will separate the data by Stimulation Time and analyse each dataset as described for Hypothesis 1 above, to test for an effect of tDCS on win learning rates. The effect in each condition (whether significant or not) will be quantified and contrasted across the two Stimulation Time conditions, to test the hypothesis that online but not offline tDCS will increase win learning rates. The same analysis approach will be used for the proportion of win-driven choices. Regarding our secondary hypothesis that online bifrontal compared to sham tDCS might improve participants' ability to adjust their learning rate to the volatility context, we will conduct exploratory analyses, using ANOVA to test the effect of tDCS on the difference in win and loss learning rates between volatile and stable blocks (i.e. dependent variables will be: Win delta LR (win learning rate in wins-volatile blocks minus win learning rate in losses-volatile blocks) and Loss delta LR (loss learning rate in losses volatile blocks minus loss learning rate in wins volatile blocks). We will test for an interaction between Stimulation Time and tDCS Condition on Win and Loss delta LRs. This will be followed up by planned contrasts within and between the online and offline tDCS Conditions, as described above for hypothesis 3.

6. Conditions and Keywords

Primary Disease or Condition Being Studied in the Trial, or the Focus of the Study
Dysphoria
Keywords
Transcranial direct current stimulation, Biological Psychiatry, Depression, Decision-making

7. Study Design

Primary Purpose
Other
Study Phase
Not Applicable
Interventional Study Model
Crossover Assignment
Model Description
Repeated-measures double-blinded research study comparing the effects of bifrontal tDCS to sham tDCS. Participants will take part in two testing sessions and will receive real and sham tDCS in counter-balanced order. Stimulation time (before vs. during task performance) will be included as a between-subject factor.
Masking
ParticipantInvestigatorOutcomes Assessor
Masking Description
A study mode on the tDCS device is used, which requires the researcher to enter a code determining whether active or sham stimulation is applied. The codes have been assigned to the participants in a randomised manner by a researcher not involved in the stimulation sessions or assessment of the outcomes. All participants receive active tDCS in one session and sham tDCS in the other session in a randomised order.
Allocation
Randomized
Enrollment
80 (Anticipated)

8. Arms, Groups, and Interventions

Arm Title
Bifrontal tDCS
Arm Type
Experimental
Arm Description
20 minutes of 2 mA transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), with the anode placed over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cathode placed over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (F3 and F4 according to the 10/20 international EEG system, respectively). The stimulation will be applied before or during task performance, depending on the condition assignment.
Arm Title
Sham tDCS
Arm Type
Sham Comparator
Arm Description
Participants receive 20 minutes of sham tDCS, with the anode placed over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cathode placed over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (F3 and F4 according to the 10/20 international EEG system, respectively). In sham tDCS, stimulation starts with 8s fade in followed by 30s direct current followed by 5s fade out followed by 870s without any stimulation. The stimulation will be applied before or during task performance, depending on the condition assignment.
Intervention Type
Device
Intervention Name(s)
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)
Intervention Description
Electric current
Primary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Change in learning rate
Description
In our previous study (Overman et al., 2021), model comparison showed that participants' behaviour on this task was best fit by a computational model combining: a Rescorla-Wagner learning rule with a Softmax function, including two separate learning rates for wins and losses; an inverse temperature parameter accounting for choice randomness; and a tendency parameter capturing a potential tendency to favour one shape over the other. Since the current study aims to replicate our previous findings, we will use the same model. The key hypothesis-driven variable of interest for analysis is the win learning rate.
Time Frame
Measure derived from task performance (40mins)
Title
Change in proportion of win-driven choices
Description
In addition to the computational model, we will also use a non-computational measure, the percentage of "win-driven choices". This is calculated from trials in which the win and loss are both associated with the same shape ("neutral" trials). What shape the participant chooses on the next trial will depend on whether s/he is more influenced by the current win or loss outcome. If the win outcome has a greater influence, the participant will choose the same shape again on the next trial. If the loss is more influential, the participant will avoid the current shape and instead choose the other shape. The proportion of "win-driven choices" is the proportion of trials in which participants choose the same shape on trial n+1 that was associated with both a win and a loss outcome on trial n. The key prediction is that this will be increased by online tDCS.
Time Frame
Measure derived from task performance (40mins)
Secondary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Change in ability to adjust learning rate to volatility
Description
As an exploratory analysis, we will test whether bifrontal tDCS might improve participants' ability to adjust their learning rates to the volatility context. The two outcome measures will be the difference in learning rates between stable and volatile blocks, e.g. the difference in win and/or loss learning rates contrasted between the wins-volatile and losses-volatile blocks (i.e. Win delta LR = win learning rate in wins-volatile blocks minus win learning rate in losses-volatile blocks; Loss delta LR = loss learning rate in losses volatile blocks minus loss learning rate in wins volatile blocks).
Time Frame
Measure derived from task performance (40mins)

10. Eligibility

Sex
All
Minimum Age & Unit of Time
18 Years
Maximum Age & Unit of Time
45 Years
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Eligibility Criteria
Inclusion Criteria: Participant is willing and able to give informed consent for participation in the study Participant has a score of >9 on Beck's Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) Fluent English-speaking Right-handed Exclusion Criteria: Currently taking psychoactive medications Personal or family history of epileptic fits or seizures Family history of extreme mood fluctuations or bipolar disorder Currently pregnant or current likelihood of becoming pregnant Significant suicidal ideation or depression requiring immediate clinical referral
Central Contact Person:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name or Official Title & Degree
Verena Sarrazin, MSc
Phone
07759903366
Email
verena.sarrazin@psych.ox.ac.uk
Overall Study Officials:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Jacinta O'Shea, PhD
Organizational Affiliation
University of Oxford
Official's Role
Principal Investigator
Facility Information:
Facility Name
FMRIB Centre, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford
City
Oxford
ZIP/Postal Code
OX3 9DU
Country
United Kingdom
Individual Site Status
Recruiting
Facility Contact:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Jacinta O'Shea, PhD
Phone
+44 (0)1865 611455
Email
jacinta.oshea@ndcn.ox.ac.uk
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Susan Field
Phone
+44 (0)1865 611450

12. IPD Sharing Statement

Plan to Share IPD
Yes
Citations:
PubMed Identifier
25730669
Citation
Browning M, Behrens TE, Jocham G, O'Reilly JX, Bishop SJ. Anxious individuals have difficulty learning the causal statistics of aversive environments. Nat Neurosci. 2015 Apr;18(4):590-6. doi: 10.1038/nn.3961. Epub 2015 Mar 2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
20434997
Citation
Fritsch B, Reis J, Martinowich K, Schambra HM, Ji Y, Cohen LG, Lu B. Direct current stimulation promotes BDNF-dependent synaptic plasticity: potential implications for motor learning. Neuron. 2010 Apr 29;66(2):198-204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.035.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
33350387
Citation
Gagne C, Zika O, Dayan P, Bishop SJ. Impaired adaptation of learning to contingency volatility in internalizing psychopathology. Elife. 2020 Dec 22;9:e61387. doi: 10.7554/eLife.61387.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
31515055
Citation
Kube T, Schwarting R, Rozenkrantz L, Glombiewski JA, Rief W. Distorted Cognitive Processes in Major Depression: A Predictive Processing Perspective. Biol Psychiatry. 2020 Mar 1;87(5):388-398. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.07.017. Epub 2019 Jul 29.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
28893377
Citation
O'Shea J, Revol P, Cousijn H, Near J, Petitet P, Jacquin-Courtois S, Johansen-Berg H, Rode G, Rossetti Y. Induced sensorimotor cortex plasticity remediates chronic treatment-resistant visual neglect. Elife. 2017 Sep 12;6:e26602. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26602.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
28976304
Citation
Pulcu E, Browning M. Affective bias as a rational response to the statistics of rewards and punishments. Elife. 2017 Oct 4;6:e27879. doi: 10.7554/eLife.27879. Erratum In: Elife. 2017 Oct 19;6:
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
32101631
Citation
Razza LB, Palumbo P, Moffa AH, Carvalho AF, Solmi M, Loo CK, Brunoni AR. A systematic review and meta-analysis on the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in depressive episodes. Depress Anxiety. 2020 Jul;37(7):594-608. doi: 10.1002/da.23004. Epub 2020 Feb 26.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19164589
Citation
Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA, Krakauer JW. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009 Feb 3;106(5):1590-5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106. Epub 2009 Jan 21.
Results Reference
background

Learn more about this trial

Effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Reward Learning in Subclinical Depression.

We'll reach out to this number within 24 hrs