search
Back to results

Optimizing Exposure Therapy With Mental Rehearsal

Primary Purpose

Anxiety Disorders, Arachnophobia

Status
Completed
Phase
Not Applicable
Locations
United States
Study Type
Interventional
Intervention
Mental Rehearsal
Exposure
Sponsored by
University of California, Los Angeles
About
Eligibility
Locations
Arms
Outcomes
Full info

About this trial

This is an interventional treatment trial for Anxiety Disorders focused on measuring anxiety, consolidation, extinction, mental rehearsal

Eligibility Criteria

18 Years - undefined (Adult, Older Adult)All SexesDoes not accept healthy volunteers

Inclusion Criteria:

  • English-speaking
  • Elevated score on Spider Phobia Questionnaire (SPQ)

Exclusion Criteria:

  • Severe allergies to bees/spiders/insects

Sites / Locations

  • University of California, Los Angeles

Arms of the Study

Arm 1

Arm 2

Arm Type

Experimental

Active Comparator

Arm Label

Mental Rehearsal

Control Rehearsal

Arm Description

Between-session rehearsal/retrieval exercises focused upon consolidating non-fear learning gained from exposures by prompting reflection of expectancy violation and rehearsal of the inhibitory association between the conditioned stimulus (i.e., spider) and unconditioned stimulus (e.g., bite/attack).

Between-session rehearsal/retrieval exercises focused upon an unrelated, recent academic experience.

Outcomes

Primary Outcome Measures

Spider Phobia Questionnaire (SPQ; Klorman et al, 1974)
31-item true/false questionnaire assessing symptoms of arachnophobia. Scores range from 0 to 31, with greater scores representing greater fear of spiders. Spider phobic individuals have obtained mean scores of 23.20 (SD = 2.90) and 23.76 (SD = 3.80) on the SPQ (Klorman et al, 1974; Murris & Merckelbach, 1996).
Behavioral Approach Test (BAT) steps
Number of test steps fully completed
SCR anticipation
Change in SCR from baseline to BAT anticipation
SCR across BAT steps
SCR during each 30-second test step fully completed
Confidence ratings
Repeated confidence ratings on a scale from 0 (no confidence) to 100 (complete confidence) recorded throughout BAT
Distress ratings
Repeated anticipatory and maximum distress ratings on a scale from 0 (no distress) to 100 (severe distress) recorded throughout BAT

Secondary Outcome Measures

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995)
21-item self-report measure that assesses severity of symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress. We use scores on the Stress subscale, which consists of 7 items measuring chronic non-specific arousal (e.g., difficulty relaxing, nervous energy, agitation, irritability). The minimum score on this subscale is 0 and the maximum score is 42 (0-14 = normal, 15-18 = mild, 19-25 = moderate, 26-33 = severe, 34+ = extremely severe).
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al, 1989)
18-item self-report measure that assesses sleep quality and disturbances over the past month. We use the global score, which sums seven component scores. Scores range from 0 to 21, with a score of 5 or greater indicating poor sleep quality.
Aerobic exercise
Brief 4-item self-report measure that assesses time spent doing scheduled and unscheduled aerobic activity during a typical week.
Surprise
Ratings of surprise on a 5-pt Likert scale (1 = not at all surprised, 5 = extremely surprised) concerning the outcome of exposures. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with greater values indicating greater surprise with the outcome of exposures.
US expectancy
Ratings of US expectancy on a 5-pt Likert scale (0 = not at all likely, 5 = extremely likely) concerning a participant's estimated likelihood of the feared outcome occurring with the same context and stimulus as in vivo exposures. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with greater values indicating greater US expectancy post-exposures.
Non-fear generalization
Ratings of US expectancy on a 5-pt Likert scale (0 = not at all likely, 5 = extremely likely) concerning a participant's estimated likelihood of the feared outcome occurring with a different spider outside the lab. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with lower values indicating greater ability to generalize safety learning.

Full Information

First Posted
April 25, 2019
Last Updated
December 16, 2019
Sponsor
University of California, Los Angeles
search

1. Study Identification

Unique Protocol Identification Number
NCT03934385
Brief Title
Optimizing Exposure Therapy With Mental Rehearsal
Official Title
Optimizing Exposure Therapy With Mental Rehearsal
Study Type
Interventional

2. Study Status

Record Verification Date
December 2019
Overall Recruitment Status
Completed
Study Start Date
October 23, 2018 (Actual)
Primary Completion Date
November 26, 2019 (Actual)
Study Completion Date
November 26, 2019 (Actual)

3. Sponsor/Collaborators

Responsible Party, by Official Title
Principal Investigator
Name of the Sponsor
University of California, Los Angeles

4. Oversight

Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Drug Product
No
Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Device Product
No
Data Monitoring Committee
Yes

5. Study Description

Brief Summary
Treatment response rates for cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) across anxiety disorders average approximately 50% post-treatment (Loerinc et al, 2015), evidencing significant 'return of fear', the re-emergence of a partially or fully extinguished fear (Rachman, 1989). Thus, recent research has amplified efforts toward improving treatment methodology in an attempt to optimize clinical outcomes. Many efforts have targeted exposure therapy, an evidence-based behavioral technique during which a patient is strategically and repeatedly exposed to his or her feared stimulus in an effort to generate new non-fear associations with that stimulus. One such effort involves mental rehearsal, where information is reinstated using either a cue from extinction training or imaginal recounting of previous successful exposures (Craske et al, 2014). Prior research has assessed the effects of mental rehearsal via reinstatement of the extinction context (i.e., treatment context) or of cues/items from the treatment context that may indicate safety (e.g., Mystkowski et al, 2006; Culver, Stoyanova, & Craske, 2011). However, this research has produced inconsistent results and contains an inherent limitation, as retrieval cues may become a safety signal and inhibit new learning (Dibbets, Havermans, & Arntz, 2008). In an effort to address these limitations, the current study recruits spider-fearful participants for a treatment trial consisting of exposures in conjunction with either a mental rehearsal intervention, or a control rehearsal intervention. The overarching goal of this project is to evaluate the extent to which a between-session, technology-guided mental rehearsal intervention may optimize exposure therapy outcomes. We also seek to evaluate potential mechanisms of mental rehearsal. Participants complete three laboratory visits, including two sessions of exposures with live spiders. Participants are randomized to either a mental rehearsal or control rehearsal condition to measure potential mechanisms and moderators of mental rehearsal. Laboratory-based assessments include measures of subjective, behavioral, and psychophysiological responses to spiders.
Detailed Description
Return of fear is the re-emergence of a partially or fully extinguished fear (Rachman, 1989). Due to relatively low treatment response rates for CBT at post-treatment (Loerinc et al, 2015), this study seeks to assess the efficacy of mental rehearsal (MR) in a different, less context-dependent manner than prior efforts (e.g., Mystkowski et al, 2006; Culver, Stoyanova, & Craske, 2011). Participants in the MR condition rehearse the new learning contingency, that is, that their feared outcome did not occur when they approached a live spider. Violation of expectancies engenders new, secondary learning that competes with the older fear memory (Craske et al, 2008; Bjork, 2003). As secondary, non-fear learning is repeatedly retrieved, the original fear memory is gradually suppressed, rendering it less recallable in the future (Bjork, 2011). Thus, repeatedly retrieving non-fear learning acquired from exposures is purported to strengthen the non-fear memory and reduce symptoms of arachnophobia. MR is conducted between sessions in an effort to reduce short-term return of fear by enhancing consolidation of non-fear learning via rehearsal efforts in multiple environments/contexts. The overall aim of the current study is to evaluate a method for enhancing the effectiveness of exposure therapy, and more specifically, to test the extent to which a novel between-session mental rehearsal intervention may optimize treatment outcomes in individuals with excessive fear of spiders. An important secondary aim is to better understand cognitive and affective mechanisms underlying benefits of mental rehearsal. The experiment consists of three sessions, spanning 8-10 days. Session 1 begins with a pre-treatment assessment consisting of self-report questionnaires and a behavioral approach test (BAT) with a live spider. During the BAT, confidence and distress ratings are obtained and psychophysiological responses (i.e., SCR) are recorded. Participants then complete a series of exposures with a live spider. At Session 2 (two to three days later), participants return to complete a second series of exposures with a live spider. At Session 3 (five to seven days later), participants complete a post-treatment assessment with self-report questionnaires and BAT, again with concurrent confidence and distress ratings and psychophysiological recordings. Between sessions, participants are randomized to mentally rehearse information from exposures (i.e., MR) or from an unrelated recent academic experience (i.e., Control). MR exercises guide participants in retrieving and consolidating learning from exposures, emphasizing the inhibitory relationship between the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US) (i.e., that approaching the spider did not result in their anticipated/feared outcome). Measures span self-report, behavioral, and psychophysiological data. Fear of spiders is assessed with self-reported symptoms and measures taken during pre- and post-treatment BATs. During each BAT, skin conductance response (SCR) serves as a physiological index of fearful arousal. Baseline SCR is collected during a two-minute period at the start of pre- and post-treatment assessments. At both BATs, anticipatory SCR is collected during a one-minute period immediately prior to starting the BAT, and SCR is then continuously recorded throughout completion of the BAT. In addition to SCR, number of steps completed (0 to 9) and repeated ratings of confidence, anticipatory distress, and maximum distress during the BAT serve as important indices of fear. Self-reported stress, sleep quality, aerobic exercise, and knowledge of spiders are assessed as potential moderators of mental rehearsal and symptom change. Post-exposure ratings of surprise, US expectancy, and generalization of non-fear learning will additionally be evaluated as treatment mechanisms.

6. Conditions and Keywords

Primary Disease or Condition Being Studied in the Trial, or the Focus of the Study
Anxiety Disorders, Arachnophobia
Keywords
anxiety, consolidation, extinction, mental rehearsal

7. Study Design

Primary Purpose
Treatment
Study Phase
Not Applicable
Interventional Study Model
Parallel Assignment
Masking
ParticipantInvestigatorOutcomes Assessor
Allocation
Randomized
Enrollment
72 (Actual)

8. Arms, Groups, and Interventions

Arm Title
Mental Rehearsal
Arm Type
Experimental
Arm Description
Between-session rehearsal/retrieval exercises focused upon consolidating non-fear learning gained from exposures by prompting reflection of expectancy violation and rehearsal of the inhibitory association between the conditioned stimulus (i.e., spider) and unconditioned stimulus (e.g., bite/attack).
Arm Title
Control Rehearsal
Arm Type
Active Comparator
Arm Description
Between-session rehearsal/retrieval exercises focused upon an unrelated, recent academic experience.
Intervention Type
Behavioral
Intervention Name(s)
Mental Rehearsal
Other Intervention Name(s)
mental retrieval
Intervention Description
After each exposure session, participants complete three rehearsal/retrieval exercises that involve viewing images of spiders and completing multiple-choice and free-response questions. Exercises involve retrieving information specific to the spider exposures, reflecting on the experience, and highlighting expectancy violation (i.e., that the participant's feared outcome did not occur).
Intervention Type
Behavioral
Intervention Name(s)
Exposure
Intervention Description
All participants complete two exposure sessions. The first set of exposures consists of ten 30-second trials hovering one's hand 3 inches over a live tarantula. The second set of exposures consists of ten 30-second trials placing one's hand inside the spider's terrarium with all five fingertips touching the bottom.
Primary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Spider Phobia Questionnaire (SPQ; Klorman et al, 1974)
Description
31-item true/false questionnaire assessing symptoms of arachnophobia. Scores range from 0 to 31, with greater scores representing greater fear of spiders. Spider phobic individuals have obtained mean scores of 23.20 (SD = 2.90) and 23.76 (SD = 3.80) on the SPQ (Klorman et al, 1974; Murris & Merckelbach, 1996).
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Title
Behavioral Approach Test (BAT) steps
Description
Number of test steps fully completed
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Title
SCR anticipation
Description
Change in SCR from baseline to BAT anticipation
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Title
SCR across BAT steps
Description
SCR during each 30-second test step fully completed
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Title
Confidence ratings
Description
Repeated confidence ratings on a scale from 0 (no confidence) to 100 (complete confidence) recorded throughout BAT
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Title
Distress ratings
Description
Repeated anticipatory and maximum distress ratings on a scale from 0 (no distress) to 100 (severe distress) recorded throughout BAT
Time Frame
Change from baseline to post-treatment (i.e., 8-10 days)
Secondary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995)
Description
21-item self-report measure that assesses severity of symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress. We use scores on the Stress subscale, which consists of 7 items measuring chronic non-specific arousal (e.g., difficulty relaxing, nervous energy, agitation, irritability). The minimum score on this subscale is 0 and the maximum score is 42 (0-14 = normal, 15-18 = mild, 19-25 = moderate, 26-33 = severe, 34+ = extremely severe).
Time Frame
Baseline
Title
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al, 1989)
Description
18-item self-report measure that assesses sleep quality and disturbances over the past month. We use the global score, which sums seven component scores. Scores range from 0 to 21, with a score of 5 or greater indicating poor sleep quality.
Time Frame
Baseline
Title
Aerobic exercise
Description
Brief 4-item self-report measure that assesses time spent doing scheduled and unscheduled aerobic activity during a typical week.
Time Frame
Baseline
Title
Surprise
Description
Ratings of surprise on a 5-pt Likert scale (1 = not at all surprised, 5 = extremely surprised) concerning the outcome of exposures. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with greater values indicating greater surprise with the outcome of exposures.
Time Frame
Session 1 and Session 2 (i.e., 3 days)
Title
US expectancy
Description
Ratings of US expectancy on a 5-pt Likert scale (0 = not at all likely, 5 = extremely likely) concerning a participant's estimated likelihood of the feared outcome occurring with the same context and stimulus as in vivo exposures. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with greater values indicating greater US expectancy post-exposures.
Time Frame
Session 1 and Session 2 (i.e., 3 days)
Title
Non-fear generalization
Description
Ratings of US expectancy on a 5-pt Likert scale (0 = not at all likely, 5 = extremely likely) concerning a participant's estimated likelihood of the feared outcome occurring with a different spider outside the lab. Scores are averaged across two exposure sessions. Scores range from 1 to 5, with lower values indicating greater ability to generalize safety learning.
Time Frame
Session 1 and Session 2 (i.e., 3 days)

10. Eligibility

Sex
All
Minimum Age & Unit of Time
18 Years
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
No
Eligibility Criteria
Inclusion Criteria: English-speaking Elevated score on Spider Phobia Questionnaire (SPQ) Exclusion Criteria: Severe allergies to bees/spiders/insects
Overall Study Officials:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Anastasia L McGlade, MA
Organizational Affiliation
University of California, Los Angeles
Official's Role
Principal Investigator
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Michelle G Craske, PhD
Organizational Affiliation
University of California, Los Angeles
Official's Role
Principal Investigator
Facility Information:
Facility Name
University of California, Los Angeles
City
Los Angeles
State/Province
California
ZIP/Postal Code
90095
Country
United States

12. IPD Sharing Statement

Citations:
PubMed Identifier
7931095
Citation
Anderson MC, Bjork RA, Bjork EL. Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994 Sep;20(5):1063-87. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.5.1063.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27818625
Citation
Aubry AV, Serrano PA, Burghardt NS. Molecular Mechanisms of Stress-Induced Increases in Fear Memory Consolidation within the Amygdala. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016 Oct 21;10:191. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00191. eCollection 2016.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.) & FABBS Foundation, Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56-64). New York, NY, US: Worth Publishers.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bjork, R. A. (2003). Interference and forgetting. In J. H. Byrne (Ed.), Encyclopedia of learning and memory, 2nd ed., (pp. 268-273). New York: Macmillan Reference USA.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bjork, R.A. (2011). On the symbiosis of learning, remembering, and forgetting. In A. S. Benjamin (Ed.), Successful remembering and successful forgetting: a Festschrift in honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 1-22). London, UK: Psychology Press.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healy, S. Kosslyn, & R. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (pp. 35-67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bloom, K. C., & Shuell, T. J. (1981). Effects of massed and distributed practice on the learning and retention of second-language vocabulary. The Journal of Educational Research, 74(4), 245-248. doi:10.1080/00220671.1981.10885317
Results Reference
background
Citation
Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1991). Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 123-140. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(91)90091-8
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16616731
Citation
Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, Maren S. Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry. 2006 Aug 15;60(4):352-60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015. Epub 2006 Apr 17.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16099088
Citation
Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog Neurobiol. 2005 Jun;76(2):99-125. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.003.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
2748771
Citation
Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989 May;28(2):193-213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
12888545
Citation
Cahill L, Gorski L, Le K. Enhanced human memory consolidation with post-learning stress: interaction with the degree of arousal at encoding. Learn Mem. 2003 Jul-Aug;10(4):270-4. doi: 10.1101/lm.62403.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
8750416
Citation
Cahill L, McGaugh JL. A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal. Conscious Cogn. 1995 Dec;4(4):410-21. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1995.1048.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
15054133
Citation
Cain CK, Blouin AM, Barad M. Adrenergic transmission facilitates extinction of conditional fear in mice. Learn Mem. 2004 Mar-Apr;11(2):179-87. doi: 10.1101/lm.71504.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17112646
Citation
Choy Y, Fyer AJ, Lipsitz JD. Treatment of specific phobia in adults. Clin Psychol Rev. 2007 Apr;27(3):266-86. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.002. Epub 2006 Nov 15.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Christopoulos, G. I., Uy, M. A., & Yap, W. J. (2016). The body and the brain: measuring skin conductance response to understand the emotional experience. Organizational Research Methods, 1-27. doi:10.1177/1094428116681073
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17765329
Citation
Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation. Trends Neurosci. 2007 Sep;30(9):464-72. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.011. Epub 2007 Aug 31. Erratum In: Trends Neurosci. 2007 Oct;30(10):489.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18005936
Citation
Craske MG, Kircanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J, Chowdhury N, Baker A. Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behav Res Ther. 2008 Jan;46(1):5-27. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003. Epub 2007 Oct 7.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24864005
Citation
Craske MG, Treanor M, Conway CC, Zbozinek T, Vervliet B. Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behav Res Ther. 2014 Jul;58:10-23. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006. Epub 2014 May 9.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
21146358
Citation
Culver NC, Stoyanova M, Craske MG. Clinical relevance of retrieval cues for attenuating context renewal of fear. J Anxiety Disord. 2011 Mar;25(2):284-92. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.002. Epub 2010 Nov 3.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18675398
Citation
Dibbets P, Havermans R, Arntz A. All we need is a cue to remember: the effect of an extinction cue on renewal. Behav Res Ther. 2008 Sep;46(9):1070-7. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.007. Epub 2008 Jun 27.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Donovan, J. J., & Radosevich, D. J. (1999). A meta-analytic review of the distribution of practice effect: Now you see it, now you don't. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 795-805. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.5.795
Results Reference
background
Citation
Driskell, J. E., Willis, R. P., & Copper, C. (1992). Effect of overlearning on retention. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 615-622. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.77.5.615
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25981173
Citation
Dunsmoor JE, Paz R. Fear Generalization and Anxiety: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry. 2015 Sep 1;78(5):336-43. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.010. Epub 2015 Apr 20.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
21282661
Citation
Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Heo S, Alves H, White SM, Wojcicki TR, Mailey E, Vieira VJ, Martin SA, Pence BD, Woods JA, McAuley E, Kramer AF. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011 Feb 15;108(7):3017-22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. Epub 2011 Jan 31.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18363421
Citation
Hofmann SG, Smits JA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008 Apr;69(4):621-32. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0415.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27437149
Citation
Hotting K, Schickert N, Kaiser J, Roder B, Schmidt-Kassow M. The Effects of Acute Physical Exercise on Memory, Peripheral BDNF, and Cortisol in Young Adults. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:6860573. doi: 10.1155/2016/6860573. Epub 2016 Jun 29.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17502911
Citation
Kalueff AV. Neurobiology of memory and anxiety: from genes to behavior. Neural Plast. 2007;2007:78171. doi: 10.1155/2007/78171. Epub 2007 Jan 10.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18276894
Citation
Karpicke JD, Roediger HL 3rd. The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science. 2008 Feb 15;319(5865):966-8. doi: 10.1126/science.1152408.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
22902568
Citation
Kircanski K, Lieberman MD, Craske MG. Feelings into words: contributions of language to exposure therapy. Psychol Sci. 2012 Oct 1;23(10):1086-91. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443830. Epub 2012 Aug 16.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Klorman, R., Weerts, T.C., Hastings, J.E., Melamed, B.G., Lang, P.J. (1974). Psychometric descriptions of some specific fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy, 5, 401-409. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80008-0
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19144740
Citation
Laine CM, Spitler KM, Mosher CP, Gothard KM. Behavioral triggers of skin conductance responses and their neural correlates in the primate amygdala. J Neurophysiol. 2009 Apr;101(4):1749-54. doi: 10.1152/jn.91110.2008. Epub 2009 Jan 14.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
10645020
Citation
Lang AJ, Craske MG. Manipulations of exposure-based therapy to reduce return of fear: a replication. Behav Res Ther. 2000 Jan;38(1):1-12. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00031-5.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Lang, A. J., Craske, M. G., & Bjork, R. A. (1999). Implications of a new theory of disuse for the treatment of emotional disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 80-94. doi:10.1093/clipsy/6.1.80
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18772260
Citation
Litman L, Davachi L. Distributed learning enhances relational memory consolidation. Learn Mem. 2008 Aug 26;15(9):711-6. doi: 10.1101/lm.1132008. Print 2008 Sep.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
26319194
Citation
Loerinc AG, Meuret AE, Twohig MP, Rosenfield D, Bluett EJ, Craske MG. Response rates for CBT for anxiety disorders: Need for standardized criteria. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015 Dec;42:72-82. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.004. Epub 2015 Aug 14.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Lovibond, S.H. & Lovibond, P.F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. (2nd Ed.) Sydney: Psychology Foundation.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25559113
Citation
McGaugh JL. Consolidating memories. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015 Jan 3;66:1-24. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-014954.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
15535740
Citation
Meeter M, Murre JM. Consolidation of long-term memory: evidence and alternatives. Psychol Bull. 2004 Nov;130(6):843-57. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.843.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27664810
Citation
Meuret AE, Rosenfield D, Bhaskara L, Auchus R, Liberzon I, Ritz T, Abelson JL. Timing matters: Endogenous cortisol mediates benefits from early-day psychotherapy. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016 Dec;74:197-202. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.008. Epub 2016 Sep 15.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25462905
Citation
Meuret AE, Trueba AF, Abelson JL, Liberzon I, Auchus R, Bhaskara L, Ritz T, Rosenfield D. High cortisol awakening response and cortisol levels moderate exposure-based psychotherapy success. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015 Jan;51:331-40. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.008. Epub 2014 Oct 16.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
8959425
Citation
Muris P, Merckelbach H. A comparison of two spider fear questionnaires. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1996 Sep;27(3):241-4. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(96)00022-5.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16942960
Citation
Mystkowski JL, Craske MG, Echiverri AM, Labus JS. Mental reinstatement of context and return of fear in spider-fearful participants. Behav Ther. 2006 Mar;37(1):49-60. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2005.04.001. Epub 2006 Feb 24.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Rachman, S. (1989). The return of fear: Review and prospect. Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 147-168. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(89)90025-1
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23589831
Citation
Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013 Apr;93(2):681-766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
10458048
Citation
Rodriguez BI, Craske MG, Mineka S, Hladek D. Context-specificity of relapse: effects of therapist and environmental context on return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1999 Sep;37(9):845-62. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00106-5.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
9682526
Citation
Rowe MK, Craske MG. Effects of an expanding-spaced vs massed exposure schedule on fear reduction and return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1998 Jul-Aug;36(7-8):701-17. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)10016-x.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
9682527
Citation
Rowe MK, Craske MG. Effects of varied-stimulus exposure training on fear reduction and return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1998 Jul-Aug;36(7-8):719-34. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)10017-1.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16856871
Citation
Soule J, Messaoudi E, Bramham CR. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and control of synaptic consolidation in the adult brain. Biochem Soc Trans. 2006 Aug;34(Pt 4):600-4. doi: 10.1042/BST0340600.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
1594723
Citation
Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992 Apr;99(2):195-231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. Erratum In: Psychol Rev 1992 Jul;99(3):582.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23537484
Citation
Vervliet B, Craske MG, Hermans D. Fear extinction and relapse: state of the art. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:215-48. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
18410984
Citation
Wolitzky-Taylor KB, Horowitz JD, Powers MB, Telch MJ. Psychological approaches in the treatment of specific phobias: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008 Jul;28(6):1021-37. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.02.007. Epub 2008 Mar 7.
Results Reference
background

Learn more about this trial

Optimizing Exposure Therapy With Mental Rehearsal

We'll reach out to this number within 24 hrs