search
Back to results

Dual-task Training in Older Adults

Primary Purpose

Healthy Aging

Status
Recruiting
Phase
Not Applicable
Locations
United States
Study Type
Interventional
Intervention
Motor Task
Cognitive-motor dual-task
Sponsored by
George Washington University
About
Eligibility
Locations
Arms
Outcomes
Full info

About this trial

This is an interventional treatment trial for Healthy Aging focused on measuring Dual-Task, Cognitive-Motor Interference, Functional Mobility, Cognition, Motor Performance

Eligibility Criteria

60 Years - 95 Years (Adult, Older Adult)All SexesAccepts Healthy Volunteers

Inclusion Criteria:

  • Between 60 and 95 Years Old
  • Self-described as generally healthy
  • Normal or corrected to normal vision
  • Able to stand on one foot for at least 3 seconds with minimal sway and no loss of balance

Exclusion Criteria:

  • Known neurologic disorder affecting mobility or cognition
  • Self-reported known moderate or greater lower extremity arthritis
  • Known disease process that affects muscle function
  • Color Blindness
  • Lower extremity pain in the previous 15 days
  • Known learning or attention deficit
  • Currently taking medication that affects attention, learning, and/or memory
  • Known Cardiovascular Disease of previous heart attack or cardiomyopathy
  • Chronic Kidney Disease
  • Severe Obesity as defined by a BMI of greater than or equal to 40 Kg/m2

Sites / Locations

  • The George Washington University, Department of Health, Human Function and Rehabilitation ScienceRecruiting

Arms of the Study

Arm 1

Arm 2

Arm 3

Arm Type

Experimental

Experimental

Active Comparator

Arm Label

Dual Task One

Dual Task Two

Control

Arm Description

Participants perform a cognitive-motor dual-task where the motor task is the same for all groups and the simultaneously performed cognitive task is simple.

Participants perform a cognitive-motor dual-task where the motor task is the same for all groups and the simultaneously performed cognitive task is complex.

Participants perform only a motor-task that is the same for all groups (no simultaneous cognitive task).

Outcomes

Primary Outcome Measures

Change in Timed Up and Go Tests in 4 week increments
Timed Up and Go, and Timed Up and Go Cognitive
Change in 10 meter walk test self-selected pace in 4 week increments
self selected pace walking over an instrumented mat (ZenoMat) both single task and while counting serial 3's
Change in Cognition in 4 week increments
Components of Cognitive Fluidity as measured by the NIH Toolbox Cognitive Battery

Secondary Outcome Measures

Balance Test Performance in 4 week increments
Single Leg Stand with and without Cognitive Task
change in 10 meter walk test fast pace in 4 week increments
Walking as fast as possible without running an instrumented mat (ZenoMat) both single task and while counting serial 3's
Change in Muscle Electromyography in 4 week increments
Surface electromyography (sEMG) will be obtained by placing wireless Delsys Trigno electrodes on the skin over the muscles of the lower extremities (tibialis anterior, lateral gastrocnemius, Rectus Femoris, Vastus Medialis, Lateral Hamstrings, and Gluteus Maximus). EMG will used to measure muscle activity during the cognitive-motor dual-task in order to measure changes in neuromuscular control strategy as subjects learn to perform the task.

Full Information

First Posted
March 5, 2022
Last Updated
April 21, 2023
Sponsor
George Washington University
search

1. Study Identification

Unique Protocol Identification Number
NCT05296551
Brief Title
Dual-task Training in Older Adults
Official Title
Examination of a Novel Cognitive-motor Weight-bearing Dual-task Intervention in Older Adults
Study Type
Interventional

2. Study Status

Record Verification Date
April 2023
Overall Recruitment Status
Recruiting
Study Start Date
March 3, 2022 (Actual)
Primary Completion Date
December 31, 2023 (Anticipated)
Study Completion Date
December 31, 2024 (Anticipated)

3. Sponsor/Collaborators

Responsible Party, by Official Title
Principal Investigator
Name of the Sponsor
George Washington University

4. Oversight

Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Drug Product
No
Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Device Product
No

5. Study Description

Brief Summary
This study seeks to understand what factors influence the capacity to perform simultaneous motor and cognitive tasks in older adults to improve movement throughout their community with the least risk of injury. To function in the real world, one needs to "walk and talk", or to move about the community while attending to their environment. Navigating a busy environment becomes increasingly difficult due to the multitude of constraints placed on the organism by both the brain and the body that are associated with aging. Resulting lack of movement causes a downward spiral; further decreasing function and increasing risk of co-morbidities. This will impose an enormous cost on our healthcare system as the elderly population continues to grow in the United States. The investigators aim to investigate both cognitive and movement changes during aging to prevent declines in functional mobility. The investigators will do this through eight weeks training of simultaneous cognitive and motor tasks (cognitive-motor training).
Detailed Description
In order to functionally move through one's community, it is vital to be able to perform simultaneous motor and cognitive tasks (cognitive-motor dual-task). This allows us to talk to another person, scan the environment for oncoming traffic, and avoid objects that are in our path when walking. This even allows high level sporting participation, such as predicting an opponents' path when sprinting and cutting to score a goal. Unfortunately, aging is associated with increased difficulty in performing cognitive-motor tasks. A classical example is when older adults stop walking in order to talk to another person. Several studies have established that an increase in age is associated with increased cognitive resources required to perform normally automatic motor tasks. Deficits in cognitive-motor dual-task capabilities have been observed not only in older adults, but also in those with neuromuscular compromise due to factors such as stroke and Parkinson's, and even with altered cognition in mild cognitive impairment and Attention Deficit Disorder. Although these early investigations suggest a relationship between physical and cognitive capabilities, little is known regarding the appropriate level of cognitive task difficulty that will result in improvements or delays in learning a motor task. Conflicting results are noted in the few studies that have investigated this line of research. Some support that a concurrent cognitive task provides a context that facilitates motor learning, while other show that cognitive load prevents individuals from fully learning the motor task . Further, there are very few studies that investigate the effect of cognitive load on the transfer of motor learning to new task conditions for weight-bearing activities. Early evidence suggests that generalizability of learning diminishes the further participants are from the initial task conditions. Interestingly, cognition has been suggested to play a vital role in motor learning, performance, and dual-task capability. Working memory capacity, or the process that allows the maintenance and manipulation of information over a short period of time, has been shown to strongly relate to the rate at which younger adults learn motor sequences, and moderately relate the motor learning of older adults. Executive function, or the properties of cognitive flexibility, problem-solving, and response maintenance, also plays a role in motor learning and performance. Decrements in executive function has been shown to precede mobility limitations, and might even predict gains in mobility from a physical intervention. Executive function may also predict a large portion of the variability when under the context of simultaneously having a cognitive load. There continues to be a lack of understanding of what cognitive capacity is required to perform simultaneous mobility and cognitive tasks with the least risk of injury, and what dose of intervention in both physical and cognitive realms are necessary to induce an improvement in function of both systems. Purpose: To determine the impact an intervention using simultaneous cognitive and motor tasks on the capacity of healthy adults to improve in functional mobility and cognition. Research Question: Does a cognitive-motor intervention impact functional mobility and cognition of healthy older adults? Following obtaining consent from the subject, testing will take place three times a week for 8 weeks. Subjects are randomly assigned to one of each of the three groups: control group, simple cognitive group, or complex cognitive group. Subjects will be informed that they are randomly assigned to one of three possible groups. Subjects will then be asked to perform either a visuomotor task only (control group), or a visuomotor task with a simultaneous cognitive task (simple cognitive group and complex cognitive group). The visuomotor task is the same regardless of group assignment. The difference in intervention between groups is based on the simultaneous task that the individual will perform: the control group will perform no cognitive task, the simple cognitive group will perform a task of counting a defined letter that appears on the screen, and the complex cognitive group will be assigned the task of counting two assigned letters that appear on the screen. The visuomotor task consists of performing a standing in-place march, lifting alternating knees to 60 degrees of hip flexion, eight cycles on each leg. A custom computer program displays a real-time video of the individual on the screen with an overlay of markers indicating computerized detection (Microsoft Kinect) of the knee joint. A prescribed marching rate is determined by an ellipse on the screen that prescribes the displacement (degrees of hip flexion) and rate of movement (speed) in which to move. Subjects will be allowed light touch for balance if required. One minute of seated rest will be provided between trials with the option of longer rest as needed. Video of the subject is saved in a de-identified format consisting of the view of the individual on the screen and the target task only during the duration of each individual trial. The cognitive task is displayed on the same screen as the motor task. Letters of different orientations and colors appear and disappear on the screen. Each of the 24 sessions will involve performing approximately 20 trials of the visuomotor and cognitive task (control group: visuomotor task only, intervention groups: visuomotor + cognitive task). Additional details regarding the visuomotor and cognitive tasks are as follows. On the first day, 13th, and 24th days, subjects will perform 20 training trials at a medium speed of the marching task (training), followed by 5 trials of varying marching speeds (testing) by altering the speed (not the amplitude) of a target ellipse that moves on the screen. Then, subjects will be asked to perform one trial of each the simple and the complex cognitive tasks without performing the marching task (cognitive task only). On days 13, and 24 days, subjects will also perform the 5 testing trials under each of the other groups' cognitive task assignment (e.g. a subject assigned to the simple cognitive task group would first perform 5 training and 5 testing trials while performing the simple cognitive task, they would then perform 5 testing trials with the complex cognitive task followed by 5 trials while only performing the motor task). During all other days of the 24 interventions, subjects will perform 20 trials of the medium speed visuomotor task, performing only the cognitive task required of their assigned group. Additionally, on the 1st,13th, and 24th day, the investigators will take height, weight, and use a scale that measures the participant's body fat percentage (by standing barefoot on the scale). Subjects will be asked to fill out questionnaires which will include information about medical, physical and social life, cognition, activity level, sleep quality and pain levels (see attached forms). Subjects will then be asked to undergo computerized testing of general cognitive function and perceived health via the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery and PROMIS (via iPad app), and a paper-based assessment of cognition (Montreal Cognitive Assessment). During the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery test, subjects will sit comfortably in a chair with their arm resting on a table and will perform four tests: the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test (FLCAT), The List Sorting Working memory Test (LSWMT), The Dimensional Change Card Sort Test (DCCST), and the Processing Speed Test (PST). The FLCAT, DCCST, and PST tests require the user to select an object on the screen using their finger as quickly as possible; the LSWMT will require no movement, but to recite objects of animals and fruits that appear on the screen of the iPad. The Subjects are free to skip any questions or tests that they would prefer not to answer or complete. A test of balance is performed asking the individual to stand on a pressure sensitive mat (Zeno Mat) to record foot pressures and amount of body sway. They will be asked to stand in place with their 1) eyes open, 2) eyes closed, and 3) performing a cognitive task while standing on each foot (single limb stance). Subjects will then be asked to perform a Timed Up and Go (TUG) test under conditions of performing a cognitive task (TUG Cognitive), and without a secondary task (TUG). The TUG test consists of moving as quickly as possible through standing from sitting, walking 3 meters, turning around, and returning three meters to return to the starting seated position. Each test is usually performed in under 30 seconds. Subjects will then be asked to perform a 10-meter walk test, where in the middle of the 8 meters of self-selected walking, they will walk over a pressure sensitive mat (ZenoMat) to determine properties of gait. The 10m walk test will be perform with and without a simultaneous cognitive task at a speed that they feel is normal for them, and then as quickly as they can walk safely. On days 1, 2, 13, and 24, the investigators will use wireless electrodes with a disposable adhesive interface will be placed over the alcohol abraded skin of lower extremity muscles in order to collect surface electromyography. Following placement of electrodes subjects will be asked to perform three maximal volitional isometric contraction (MVIC) for each muscle. Each MVIC is achieved by the experimenter applying manual resistance in serial with a hand-held dynamometer at the distal most portion of the segment (leg) to which the muscle attaches. The subject is then provided with verbal encouragement to move the limb being tested in the primary direction of muscle movement (eg. For the quadriceps muscles, resistance is applied to the distal tibia at the level of just proximal to the malleoli during knee extension). Between each maximal effort, 1-min rest will be provided to prevent fatigue.

6. Conditions and Keywords

Primary Disease or Condition Being Studied in the Trial, or the Focus of the Study
Healthy Aging
Keywords
Dual-Task, Cognitive-Motor Interference, Functional Mobility, Cognition, Motor Performance

7. Study Design

Primary Purpose
Treatment
Study Phase
Not Applicable
Interventional Study Model
Parallel Assignment
Model Description
Subjects are block randomized into 3 parallel groups.
Masking
None (Open Label)
Allocation
Randomized
Enrollment
30 (Anticipated)

8. Arms, Groups, and Interventions

Arm Title
Dual Task One
Arm Type
Experimental
Arm Description
Participants perform a cognitive-motor dual-task where the motor task is the same for all groups and the simultaneously performed cognitive task is simple.
Arm Title
Dual Task Two
Arm Type
Experimental
Arm Description
Participants perform a cognitive-motor dual-task where the motor task is the same for all groups and the simultaneously performed cognitive task is complex.
Arm Title
Control
Arm Type
Active Comparator
Arm Description
Participants perform only a motor-task that is the same for all groups (no simultaneous cognitive task).
Intervention Type
Other
Intervention Name(s)
Motor Task
Intervention Description
A visuomotor task is presented where an individual must match their knee to an ellipse that moves up and down on a screen, resulting in a marching-in-place motion at a specified frequency.
Intervention Type
Other
Intervention Name(s)
Cognitive-motor dual-task
Intervention Description
While performing the "Motor Task" intervention, letters of different colors and orientation randomly appear one at a time on the screen. During the Cognitive-motor dual-task, individuals are instructed to count the number of times that either 1 (simple cognitive task), or 2 (complex cognitive task) characters appear on the screen during the motor task.
Primary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Change in Timed Up and Go Tests in 4 week increments
Description
Timed Up and Go, and Timed Up and Go Cognitive
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4, and Week 8
Title
Change in 10 meter walk test self-selected pace in 4 week increments
Description
self selected pace walking over an instrumented mat (ZenoMat) both single task and while counting serial 3's
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4, and Week 8
Title
Change in Cognition in 4 week increments
Description
Components of Cognitive Fluidity as measured by the NIH Toolbox Cognitive Battery
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4, and Week 8
Secondary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Balance Test Performance in 4 week increments
Description
Single Leg Stand with and without Cognitive Task
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4, and Week 8
Title
change in 10 meter walk test fast pace in 4 week increments
Description
Walking as fast as possible without running an instrumented mat (ZenoMat) both single task and while counting serial 3's
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4, and Week 8
Title
Change in Muscle Electromyography in 4 week increments
Description
Surface electromyography (sEMG) will be obtained by placing wireless Delsys Trigno electrodes on the skin over the muscles of the lower extremities (tibialis anterior, lateral gastrocnemius, Rectus Femoris, Vastus Medialis, Lateral Hamstrings, and Gluteus Maximus). EMG will used to measure muscle activity during the cognitive-motor dual-task in order to measure changes in neuromuscular control strategy as subjects learn to perform the task.
Time Frame
Baseline, Week 4 and Week 8

10. Eligibility

Sex
All
Minimum Age & Unit of Time
60 Years
Maximum Age & Unit of Time
95 Years
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Eligibility Criteria
Inclusion Criteria: Between 60 and 95 Years Old Self-described as generally healthy Normal or corrected to normal vision Able to stand on one foot for at least 3 seconds with minimal sway and no loss of balance Exclusion Criteria: Known neurologic disorder affecting mobility or cognition Self-reported known moderate or greater lower extremity arthritis Known disease process that affects muscle function Color Blindness Lower extremity pain in the previous 15 days Known learning or attention deficit Currently taking medication that affects attention, learning, and/or memory Known Cardiovascular Disease of previous heart attack or cardiomyopathy Chronic Kidney Disease Severe Obesity as defined by a BMI of greater than or equal to 40 Kg/m2
Central Contact Person:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name or Official Title & Degree
Keith Cole, DPT, Ph. D.
Phone
202-994-0423
Email
keithcole@gwu.edu
Overall Study Officials:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Keith Cole, DPT, Ph. D.
Organizational Affiliation
The George Washington University
Official's Role
Principal Investigator
Facility Information:
Facility Name
The George Washington University, Department of Health, Human Function and Rehabilitation Science
City
Washington
State/Province
District of Columbia
ZIP/Postal Code
20006
Country
United States
Individual Site Status
Recruiting
Facility Contact:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Keith Cole, DPT, Ph.D.
Phone
202-994-0423
Email
keithcole@email.gwu.edu

12. IPD Sharing Statement

Plan to Share IPD
No
Citations:
PubMed Identifier
12524257
Citation
American Thoracic Society; American College of Chest Physicians. ATS/ACCP Statement on cardiopulmonary exercise testing. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2003 Jan 15;167(2):211-77. doi: 10.1164/rccm.167.2.211. No abstract available. Erratum In: Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2003 May 15;1451-2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27363617
Citation
Alcock L, Galna B, Lord S, Rochester L. Characterisation of foot clearance during gait in people with early Parkinson׳s disease: Deficits associated with a dual task. J Biomech. 2016 Sep 6;49(13):2763-2769. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.06.007. Epub 2016 Jun 15.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16794878
Citation
Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006 Mar;16(1):17-42. doi: 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24684316
Citation
Alves FD, Souza GC, Biolo A, Clausell N. Comparison of two bioelectrical impedance devices and dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry to evaluate body composition in heart failure. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2014 Dec;27(6):632-8. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12218. Epub 2014 Mar 29.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23032876
Citation
Amboni M, Barone P, Iuppariello L, Lista I, Tranfaglia R, Fasano A, Picillo M, Vitale C, Santangelo G, Agosti V, Iavarone A, Sorrentino G. Gait patterns in Parkinsonian patients with or without mild cognitive impairment. Mov Disord. 2012 Oct;27(12):1536-43. doi: 10.1002/mds.25165. Epub 2012 Oct 2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
3809575
Citation
Baddeley A, Logie R, Bressi S, Della Sala S, Spinnler H. Dementia and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1986 Nov;38(4):603-18. doi: 10.1080/14640748608401616. No abstract available.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23187040
Citation
Baetens T, De Kegel A, Palmans T, Oostra K, Vanderstraeten G, Cambier D. Gait analysis with cognitive-motor dual tasks to distinguish fallers from nonfallers among rehabilitating stroke patients. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013 Apr;94(4):680-6. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2012.11.023. Epub 2012 Nov 24.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
20585013
Citation
Balady GJ, Arena R, Sietsema K, Myers J, Coke L, Fletcher GF, Forman D, Franklin B, Guazzi M, Gulati M, Keteyian SJ, Lavie CJ, Macko R, Mancini D, Milani RV; American Heart Association Exercise, Cardiac Rehabilitation, and Prevention Committee of the Council on Clinical Cardiology; Council on Epidemiology and Prevention; Council on Peripheral Vascular Disease; Interdisciplinary Council on Quality of Care and Outcomes Research. Clinician's Guide to cardiopulmonary exercise testing in adults: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2010 Jul 13;122(2):191-225. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e3181e52e69. Epub 2010 Jun 28. No abstract available.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
3087938
Citation
Beaver WL, Wasserman K, Whipp BJ. A new method for detecting anaerobic threshold by gas exchange. J Appl Physiol (1985). 1986 Jun;60(6):2020-7. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1986.60.6.2020.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24133292
Citation
Bedard P, Song JH. Attention modulates generalization of visuomotor adaptation. J Vis. 2013 Oct 16;13(12):12. doi: 10.1167/13.12.12.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24522972
Citation
Blumen HM, Holtzer R, Brown LL, Gazes Y, Verghese J. Behavioral and neural correlates of imagined walking and walking-while-talking in the elderly. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014 Aug;35(8):4090-104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22461. Epub 2014 Feb 12.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19726728
Citation
Bo J, Borza V, Seidler RD. Age-related declines in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in explicit motor sequence learning. J Neurophysiol. 2009 Nov;102(5):2744-54. doi: 10.1152/jn.00393.2009. Epub 2009 Sep 2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19357338
Citation
Bo J, Seidler RD. Visuospatial working memory capacity predicts the organization of acquired explicit motor sequences. J Neurophysiol. 2009 Jun;101(6):3116-25. doi: 10.1152/jn.00006.2009. Epub 2009 Apr 8.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
11535954
Citation
Bolanowski M, Nilsson BE. Assessment of human body composition using dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry and bioelectrical impedance analysis. Med Sci Monit. 2001 Sep-Oct;7(5):1029-33.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19969461
Citation
Brauer SG, Morris ME. Can people with Parkinson's disease improve dual tasking when walking? Gait Posture. 2010 Feb;31(2):229-33. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.10.011. Epub 2009 Dec 6.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
12625657
Citation
Cockburn J, Haggard P, Cock J, Fordham C. Changing patterns of cognitive-motor interference (CMI) over time during recovery from stroke. Clin Rehabil. 2003 Mar;17(2):167-73. doi: 10.1191/0269215503cr597oa.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19527695
Citation
Dennis A, Dawes H, Elsworth C, Collett J, Howells K, Wade DT, Izadi H, Cockburn J. Fast walking under cognitive-motor interference conditions in chronic stroke. Brain Res. 2009 Sep 1;1287:104-10. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.023. Epub 2009 Jun 13.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23949972
Citation
Doi T, Makizako H, Shimada H, Park H, Tsutsumimoto K, Uemura K, Suzuki T. Brain activation during dual-task walking and executive function among older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a fNIRS study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2013 Oct;25(5):539-44. doi: 10.1007/s40520-013-0119-5. Epub 2013 Aug 15.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
19439578
Citation
Elovainio M, Kivimaki M, Ferrie JE, Gimeno D, De Vogli R, Virtanen M, Vahtera J, Brunner EJ, Marmot MG, Singh-Manoux A. Physical and cognitive function in midlife: reciprocal effects? A 5-year follow-up of the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009 Jun;63(6):468-73. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.081505.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
16109493
Citation
Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ, Wadhwa R, Bherer L, Peterson MS, Scalf PE, Kramer AF. Neural correlates of dual-task performance after minimizing task-preparation. Neuroimage. 2005 Dec;28(4):967-79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.047. Epub 2005 Aug 16.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
26079569
Citation
Fritz NE, Cheek FM, Nichols-Larsen DS. Motor-Cognitive Dual-Task Training in Persons With Neurologic Disorders: A Systematic Review. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2015 Jul;39(3):142-53. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000090.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23103242
Citation
Galna B, Lord S, Rochester L. Is gait variability reliable in older adults and Parkinson's disease? Towards an optimal testing protocol. Gait Posture. 2013 Apr;37(4):580-5. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.09.025. Epub 2012 Oct 25.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
22886044
Citation
Goh HT, Sullivan KJ, Gordon J, Wulf G, Winstein CJ. Dual-task practice enhances motor learning: a preliminary investigation. Exp Brain Res. 2012 Oct;222(3):201-10. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3206-5. Epub 2012 Aug 12.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17874957
Citation
Gothe K, Oberauer K, Kliegl R. Age differences in dual-task performance after practice. Psychol Aging. 2007 Sep;22(3):596-606. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.596.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24521364
Citation
Gothe NP, Fanning J, Awick E, Chung D, Wojcicki TR, Olson EA, Mullen SP, Voss M, Erickson KI, Kramer AF, McAuley E. Executive function processes predict mobility outcomes in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014 Feb;62(2):285-90. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12654. Epub 2014 Jan 21.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
11532885
Citation
Herath P, Klingberg T, Young J, Amunts K, Roland P. Neural correlates of dual task interference can be dissociated from those of divided attention: an fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2001 Sep;11(9):796-805. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.796.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24768615
Citation
Jacobs JV, Nutt JG, Carlson-Kuhta P, Allen R, Horak FB. Dual tasking during postural stepping responses increases falls but not freezing in people with Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014 Jul;20(7):779-81. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.04.001. Epub 2014 Apr 14.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
10870864
Citation
Jones AM, Carter H. The effect of endurance training on parameters of aerobic fitness. Sports Med. 2000 Jun;29(6):373-86. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200029060-00001.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24398644
Citation
Kafri MW, Potter JF, Myint PK. Multi-frequency bioelectrical impedance analysis for assessing fat mass and fat-free mass in stroke or transient ischaemic attack patients. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2014 Jun;68(6):677-82. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2013.266. Epub 2014 Jan 8.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17401735
Citation
Leitner Y, Barak R, Giladi N, Peretz C, Eshel R, Gruendlinger L, Hausdorff JM. Gait in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder : effects of methylphenidate and dual tasking. J Neurol. 2007 Oct;254(10):1330-8. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0522-3. Epub 2007 Apr 2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
9057736
Citation
Lundin-Olsson L, Nyberg L, Gustafson Y. "Stops walking when talking" as a predictor of falls in elderly people. Lancet. 1997 Mar 1;349(9052):617. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)24009-2. No abstract available.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
22694665
Citation
Makizako H, Doi T, Shimada H, Yoshida D, Takayama Y, Suzuki T. Relationship between dual-task performance and neurocognitive measures in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2013 Apr;13(2):314-21. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00898.x. Epub 2012 Jun 14.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23211228
Citation
Makizako H, Doi T, Shimada H, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, Uemura K, Suzuki T. Does a multicomponent exercise program improve dual-task performance in amnestic mild cognitive impairment? A randomized controlled trial. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2012 Dec;24(6):640-6. doi: 10.3275/8760. Epub 2012 Nov 26.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27256077
Citation
Marques NR, Hallal CZ, Spinoso DH, Morcelli MH, Crozara LF, Goncalves M. Applying different mathematical variability methods to identify older fallers and non-fallers using gait variability data. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2017 Jun;29(3):473-481. doi: 10.1007/s40520-016-0592-8. Epub 2016 Jun 2.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
21940172
Citation
Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, Borrie M, Gopaul K, Montero-Odasso M. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait Posture. 2012 Jan;35(1):96-100. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.08.014. Epub 2011 Sep 22.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
12201803
Citation
O'Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther. 2002 Sep;82(9):888-97.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25972801
Citation
Plummer P, Eskes G. Measuring treatment effects on dual-task performance: a framework for research and clinical practice. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015 Apr 28;9:225. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00225. eCollection 2015.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23973751
Citation
Plummer P, Eskes G, Wallace S, Giuffrida C, Fraas M, Campbell G, Clifton KL, Skidmore ER; American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine Stroke Networking Group Cognition Task Force. Cognitive-motor interference during functional mobility after stroke: state of the science and implications for future research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2013 Dec;94(12):2565-2574.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2013.08.002. Epub 2013 Aug 20.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
29738743
Citation
Plummer P, Iyigun G. Effects of Physical Exercise Interventions on Dual-Task Gait Speed Following Stroke: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2018 Dec;99(12):2548-2560. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2018.04.009. Epub 2018 May 5.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
26050072
Citation
Plummer P, Osborne MB. What Are We Attempting to Improve When We Train Dual-Task Performance? J Neurol Phys Ther. 2015 Jul;39(3):154-5. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000097. No abstract available.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25721432
Citation
Plummer P, Zukowski LA, Giuliani C, Hall AM, Zurakowski D. Effects of Physical Exercise Interventions on Gait-Related Dual-Task Interference in Older Adults: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Gerontology. 2015;62(1):94-117. doi: 10.1159/000371577. Epub 2015 Feb 19.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
17727104
Citation
Roche RA, Commins S, Agnew F, Cassidy S, Corapi K, Leibbrand S, Lipson Z, Rickard J, Sorohan J, Wynne C, O'Mara SM. Concurrent task performance enhances low-level visuomotor learning. Percept Psychophys. 2007 May;69(4):513-22. doi: 10.3758/bf03193908.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
24857405
Citation
Sanli EA, Lee TD. What roles do errors serve in motor skill learning? An examination of two theoretical predictions. J Mot Behav. 2014;46(5):329-37. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2014.913544. Epub 2014 May 23.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
22350284
Citation
Shorer Z, Becker B, Jacobi-Polishook T, Oddsson L, Melzer I. Postural control among children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in single and dual conditions. Eur J Pediatr. 2012 Jul;171(7):1087-94. doi: 10.1007/s00431-012-1695-7. Epub 2012 Feb 16.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
25501806
Citation
Song JH, Bedard P. Paradoxical benefits of dual-task contexts for visuomotor memory. Psychol Sci. 2015 Feb;26(2):148-58. doi: 10.1177/0956797614557868. Epub 2014 Dec 10.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
22168474
Citation
Strobach T, Frensch P, Muller H, Schubert T. Age- and practice-related influences on dual-task costs and compensation mechanisms under optimal conditions of dual-task performance. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2012;19(1-2):222-47. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.630973. Epub 2011 Dec 14.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
21947746
Citation
Strobach T, Frensch PA, Soutschek A, Schubert T. Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills. Psychol Res. 2012 Nov;76(6):794-811. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0381-0. Epub 2011 Sep 27.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
2021122
Citation
Svendsen OL, Haarbo J, Heitmann BL, Gotfredsen A, Christiansen C. Measurement of body fat in elderly subjects by dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry, bioelectrical impedance, and anthropometry. Am J Clin Nutr. 1991 May;53(5):1117-23. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/53.5.1117.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
12127181
Citation
Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002 Aug;16(1):1-14. doi: 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00156-4.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
23063842
Citation
Wu T, Liu J, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Cerebellum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processing. Neuroimage. 2013 Jan 15;65:466-75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.004. Epub 2012 Oct 11.
Results Reference
background
Citation
(2004). Oxygen Uptake Kinetics in Sport, Exercise and Medicine, Routledge.
Results Reference
background
Citation
Dalleck, L.C. and J.S. Tischendorf. Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription (ACSM). Encyclopedia of Lifestyle Medicine & Health, SAGE Publications, Inc.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
27548008
Citation
Wajda DA, Mirelman A, Hausdorff JM, Sosnoff JJ. Intervention modalities for targeting cognitive-motor interference in individuals with neurodegenerative disease: a systematic review. Expert Rev Neurother. 2017 Mar;17(3):251-261. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2016.1227704. Epub 2016 Sep 12.
Results Reference
background
PubMed Identifier
30600778
Citation
Cole KR, Shields RK. Age and Cognitive Stress Influences Motor Skill Acquisition, Consolidation, and Dual-Task Effect in Humans. J Mot Behav. 2019;51(6):622-639. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2018.1547893. Epub 2019 Jan 2.
Results Reference
background

Learn more about this trial

Dual-task Training in Older Adults

We'll reach out to this number within 24 hrs