search
Back to results

Retrieval-Based Word Learning in Developmental Language Disorder

Primary Purpose

Developmental Language Disorder, Specific Language Impairment, Language Development

Status
Active
Phase
Not Applicable
Locations
United States
Study Type
Interventional
Intervention
Retrieval-based word learning
Sponsored by
Purdue University
About
Eligibility
Locations
Arms
Outcomes
Full info

About this trial

This is an interventional basic science trial for Developmental Language Disorder focused on measuring Word learning, Word retrieval, Preschool-aged children

Eligibility Criteria

48 Months - 71 Months (Child)All SexesAccepts Healthy Volunteers

Inclusion Criteria:

  • a significant deficit in language ability (language test score below cutoff for best sensitivity/specificity) or documented age-appropriate language ability.
  • normal hearing
  • no evidence of neurological damage or disease
  • scores on tests of nonverbal intelligence above the intellectual disability range
  • not within Autistic range on Autism screening test
  • native English speaker (can be bilingual)

Exclusion Criteria

  • failed hearing screening
  • known neurological damage or disease
  • scores on tests of nonverbal intelligence below the intellectual disability range (standard score less than 75)
  • autism spectrum disorder
  • non-native English speaker

Sites / Locations

  • Purdue University

Arms of the Study

Arm 1

Arm Type

Experimental

Arm Label

Retrieval practice during word learning: expanding schedule

Arm Description

Each child will learn 8 novel nouns referring to unfamiliar plants and animals ("nepp") and a related "meaning" ("a nepp likes rain") . Four nouns will be learned in an expanding spaced retrieval practice condition; four will be learned in a "standard" spaced retrieval practice condition. In the "standard" spaced retrieval practice condition, the number of other words intervening between hearing the target and an attempt to retrieve it will increase from 0 to 3 words. In the expanding spaced retrieval practice condition, the number of other words intervening between hearing the target and an attempt to retrieve it will increase gradually from 0 to 1 to 3 words.

Outcomes

Primary Outcome Measures

Word form recall accuracy (number of words correctly recalled)
Child is asked to recall and say the word associated with each novel referent.
Word form recall accuracy (number of words correctly recalled)
Child is asked to recall and say the word associated with each novel referent.
Word meaning recall accuracy (number of semantic associations correctly recalled)
Child is asked to recall and say semantic information associated with the novel referent.
Word meaning recall accuracy (number of semantic associations correctly recalled)
Child is asked to recall and say semantic information associated with the novel referent.
Word recognition (number of words accurately identified)
Upon hearing a novel word, the child is asked to indicate (by pointing) which picture among 4 options is referred to.

Secondary Outcome Measures

Number of correctly identified words on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 5th Ed.
Upon hearing a word, the child is asked to indicate (by pointing) which picture among 4 options is referred to.
Years of maternal education
Primary care-giver provides information regarding highest academic degree attained, converted to years of education

Full Information

First Posted
April 5, 2022
Last Updated
August 3, 2023
Sponsor
Purdue University
Collaborators
National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD)
search

1. Study Identification

Unique Protocol Identification Number
NCT05325333
Brief Title
Retrieval-Based Word Learning in Developmental Language Disorder
Official Title
Retrieval-Based Word Learning in Developmental Language Disorder
Study Type
Interventional

2. Study Status

Record Verification Date
August 2023
Overall Recruitment Status
Active, not recruiting
Study Start Date
March 16, 2022 (Actual)
Primary Completion Date
July 31, 2026 (Anticipated)
Study Completion Date
July 31, 2026 (Anticipated)

3. Sponsor/Collaborators

Responsible Party, by Official Title
Principal Investigator
Name of the Sponsor
Purdue University
Collaborators
National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD)

4. Oversight

Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Drug Product
No
Studies a U.S. FDA-regulated Device Product
No
Data Monitoring Committee
No

5. Study Description

Brief Summary
Children with developmental language disorder (DLD; also referred to as specific language impairment) experience a significant deficit in language ability that is longstanding and harmful to the children's academic, social, and eventual economic wellbeing. Word learning is one of the principal weaknesses in these children. This project focuses on the word learning abilities of four- and five-year-old children with DLD. The goal of the project is to build on our previous work to determine whether, as we have found thus far, special benefits accrue when these children must frequently recall newly introduced words during the course of learning. In this first of a series of studies, we seek to increase the children's absolute levels of learning while maintaining the advantage that repeated retrieval holds over comparison methods of learning.
Detailed Description
Although much of the DLD literature has emphasized morphosyntax, those children with morphosyntactic weaknesses also have major deficits in vocabulary (McGregor et al., 2012), and sophisticated statistical approaches applied to large databases find no basis for treating lexical and morphosyntactic skill as separate dimensions (Tomblin et al., 2014). Longitudinal studies show that individuals with DLD fall further behind their peers in vocabulary ability from preschool to 21 years of age (Rice & Hoffman, 2015). Vocabulary scores for adults with a history of DLD are significantly lower than those of other adults, with most scores falling within a clinical range (Tomblin et al., 1992). Many studies have shown that the vocabularies of children with DLD have less breadth and depth than those of their same-age peers (Kail & Leonard, 1986; Krzemien et al., 2020; McGregor et al., 2002; McGregor, Oleson et al., 2013; McGregor & Waxman, 1998). To examine the dynamic aspects of word learning in these children, investigators have often presented the children with novel words to be learned, carefully controlling for the frequency and type of input provided. These studies have shown that children with DLD require more encounters with a word before learning takes place, with weaknesses evident in both semantic and phonological encoding (Alt, 2011; Alt & Plante, 2006; Alt et al., 2004; Gray, 2003, 2004; Gray et al., 2014; Leonard et al., 1982; McGregor, Licandro et al., 2013; Rice et al., 1994). Novel word learning weaknesses have been documented for nouns, verbs, and adjectives (Kan & Windsor, 2010; Oetting et al., 1995; Skipp et al., 2002; Rice et al., 1990; Windfuhr et al., 2002). In our previous work, we added retrieval practice to the standard novel word learning protocol. In previous studies of word learning by children with DLD, retrieval was used only as an assessment tool - to determine when or if a child had met a particular criterion in learning a set of new words. Based on our understanding of the growing literature on retrieval effects in the field of cognitive psychology, we introduced retrieval practice as a word learning facilitator, an application that has both theoretical and clinical implications. We tested the hypothesis that the act of retrieving new words significantly assists the learning of these words by children with DLD by markedly increasing long-term retention relative to levels seen through other types of word learning, even when the total exposure to the words is the same or less. Based on very encouraging findings, we continue our research with the goal of moving the use of retrieval closer toward becoming a procedure that can be incorporated into real-word clinical and educational settings. The rationale behind our work is grounded in findings from the memory literature that, following an initial study period, repeated practice in retrieving the material results in greater long-term retention than additional study of that material (Karpicke, 2012; Karpicke & Blunt, 2011; Karpicke & Roediger, 2007b, 2008; Roediger & Karpicke, 2006; see meta-analysis in Rowland, 2014). The benefits are dramatic; in these studies, long-term retention is often 50% to 150% greater in the repeated retrieval condition. This effect has been seen for a wide range of material, from simple paired associates (e.g., knee-cloud) to fictional stories. The benefits of repeated retrieval can be further enhanced when there is spacing between retrieval attempts. Spacing can be defined in terms of time but also in terms of intervening material. Ideal spacing is one which is "effortful" but short enough to prevent forgetting. Repeated retrieval that occurs immediately after study can be helpful but does not yield the long-term recall seen for repeated spaced retrieval (Karpicke & Roediger, 2007a). Our application of repeated spaced retrieval to the word learning of children with DLD has produced a rather clear set of results. The general method has been the same across experiments. Children with DLD age 4;0 to 5;11 and their same-age peers with typical language development (TD) learned sets of novel words (e.g., /nɛp/, /zogi/, /kudɪp/) in two sessions held on consecutive days and were then tested immediately after the learning period and again one week later. A within-participant design was used where each child learned half the novel words in a repeated spaced retrieval (RSR) condition and the other half in a comparison learning condition. Both conditions included "study" trials, where the child saw the picture and heard both the word form (e.g., /nɛp/) and, for studies employing words representing nouns, the word's "meaning" (e.g., A /nɛp/ likes rain). For conditions that included retrieval trials, the child saw the picture and was asked, for example, "What's this called? What do we call this?" and then "And what does this one like? What does it like?" For the RSR conditions in these studies, spacing was created by having two or three intervening words (depending on the experiment) between a word's previous study trial and its retrieval trial. Testing after the learning phase involved word form recall, meaning recall, and recognition (selecting the correct picture from an array, e.g., "Where's the nep?"). Our main findings were that: (1) children learned more novel words representing nouns (names of exotic plants and animals) in a RSR condition than in a repeated study condition that provided the same amount of exposure to the words; (2) children learned more novel nouns in a RSR condition than in an immediate retrieval condition that provided not only the same amount of exposure but also the same number of retrieval opportunities; (3) RSR produced better learning even when exposure was reduced relative to the comparison condition; (4) RSR was similarly advantageous when children learned novel words referring to adjectives that required the children to generalize to new objects showing the same attribute; (5) RSR advantages were seen not only in behavioral tasks but also at the neural level as captured through event-related potentials (ERPs); (6) the word learning gap between children with DLD and their TD age mates was smaller for words in the RSR condition than for words in the comparison conditions; and (7) the recall of the children with DLD from immediate testing to one-week testing was very stable and indistinguishable from that of their TD peers. Despite these encouraging findings, much remains to be learned about RSR and its effects on the learning of children with DLD and how this learning compares to that of children who are developing language on schedule. One important question is the absolute level of learning achieved through RSR. In our studies to date, in the more optimal condition of RSR, children with DLD have shown high accuracy levels on meaning recall (e.g., recalling that a /pobɪk/ likes birds) and recognition (e.g., pointing to the picture of the /pobɪk/ out of an array of four pictures). However, for word form recall (recalling the phonetic form of the word /pobɪk/), the children with DLD have recalled only about 55% of the novel words on average - this despite recalling twice as many word forms as in the comparison conditions. If the children were successful in recalling the word immediately after the learning period, they also recalled the word one week later. The main obstacle appeared to be initial word form encoding (Haebig, Leonard et al. 2019). In the current study, we seek to improve word form recall by the children through modifications that may improve encoding in particular. The principal means by which we hope to improve encoding and, as a result, subsequent recall, is to create a more gradual spacing of retrieval as the learning period proceeds. In our most frequently used "standard" procedure (Leonard, Karpicke, et al., 2019), the early trials involved the children being asked to recall the word form and meaning immediately after the study trial for the same word. For subsequent recall trials, three other words intervened from the last time the child heard the word and when the child was asked to recall it. Subsequent retrieval trials involved the same degree of spacing. In the current study, we will compare this standard procedure with an expanding retrieval schedule (e.g., Karpicke & Roediger, 2007a; Landauer & Bjork, 1978). As in the standard procedure, the first retrieval trial is an immediate retrieval trial. Then, the spacing between study and subsequent retrieval of the word is initially narrow -- one intervening word - and expands more gradually to three intervening words. The words in the two learning conditions - standard retrieval and expanding retrieval - will be presented in separate sets. The sets will be counterbalanced in order. Each set will involve two learning sessions held on consecutive days with a recall test administered five minutes after the second learning session, followed by another recall test one week later. The second set will begin the next week, following the same scheme as the first set. Four novel words will be learned in each set. The words will match those used in our previous studies on novel nouns (e.g., /nɛp/, referring to an unusual animal; /bog/ referring to an unusual plant). Colored photographs appearing on a laptop screen will be the visual referents for these words. The novel nouns serve as the word forms; we will also associate each word with something the referent "likes" (although what it likes will not be shown on the photograph. This will serve as the "meaning" of the word. During each study trial, the novel word will be mentioned three times and its meaning will be mentioned once, as in: "This is a /nɛp/. It's a /nɛp/. A /nɛp/ likes rain". For conditions that include retrieval trials, the child will see the picture and will be asked, "What's this called? What do we call this?" and then "And what does this one like? What does it like?" Audio stimuli will be pre-recorded and presented along with the visual stimuli (scanned photographs) on the laptop. The experimenter will manually move the program from one trial to the next to ensure that the child is attending to each trial. The novel words will consist of monosyllabic (consonant-vowel-consonant) shapes, thus conforming to the novel words used in our initial studies with the standard procedure. The novel words assigned to the two conditions will be matched according to phonotactic probability and neighborhood density (based on Storkel & Hoover, 2010). The two learning sessions within each set will be identical. For the set constituting the standard condition, each novel word will be presented initially in a study trial, followed by an immediate retrieval trial, followed by another study trial. We refer to such retrieval trials as "0" retrieval trials, reflecting the fact that there are zero words intervening between a word's study trial and its retrieval trial. After each novel word has had a 0 trial, all subsequent retrieval trials will occur after three other words had intervened since the last time the word had appeared in a study trial. Such trials will be followed by a study trial for the same word. We refer to these retrieval trials as "3" retrieval trials, again, reflecting the number of intervening words used. The retrieval schedule for each day for the words in the standard condition will be 0-3-3-3-3. However, after the second 3 trial of the four words, we will insert one study trial for each word. This study trial is intended as a "refresher" under the assumption that some words will not yet be recalled at that point. The insertion of these study trials do not change the 3 spacing between a word's retrieval trial and its previous study trial. The next day will likewise have the 0-3-3-3-3 schedule with a study trial after the second 3 trial. For the set constituting the expanding retrieval condition, each word will first occur in a study trial, followed by an immediate retrieval trial, and then another study trial. The next two retrieval trials will occur after one other novel word had intervened since the word's most recent study trial. These retrieval trials will be followed by a study trial. We refer to these retrieval trials as "1" retrieval trials, as there was one intervening word. The two remaining retrieval trials for words in this condition will be 3 retrieval trials, with each retrieval trial followed by a study trial for the same word. The schedule for each day, then, will be 0-1-1-3-3. After the second 1 retrieval trial for the four words in the set, a study trial will be presented for each word as a "refresher," as was done for the words in the standard condition. With this design, the two learning conditions will provide the same number of exposures to the word forms and meanings, and the same number of retrieval opportunities throughout the two-day learning period. The two conditions will differ only in the retrieval schedule - 0-3-3-3-3 versus 0-1-1-3-3. For each set, after the child has completed the learning session of the second day, a five-minute break will be given. Then a recall test of each word form and meaning will be administered. Two items for each word form and meaning will be used, with the second item for each word presented only after all four words had been tested once. Prompts used for the recall items will be identical to those used in the retrieval trials. One week later, the recall test will be re-administered. This test will be followed by a recognition test. The photo of each novel word referent will be shown along with the referents of the other three referents (in an array of four photos) and the child will be asked, "Show me the e.g., /nɛp/." More details are provided in the Outcome Measures section. We hypothesize that the use of 1 retrieval trials prior to proceeding the 3 retrieval trials will increase the children's ability to encode the novel word forms and meanings. Accordingly, we expect that recall and recognition scores will be higher for words in the expanding 0-1-1-3-3 condition than in the standard 0-3-3-3-3 condition. This is a rather stringent test given that our previous work has shown that the standard condition was more successful than comparison conditions that did not involve spacing. Nevertheless, it is necessary to attempt to improve on the standard condition given that it did not enable children to learn more than approximately half of the words employed. We expect that the expanding condition will lead to better results than the standard condition for both the children with DLD and the children with TD. In our earlier work, group differences were often seen favoring the TD children for the standard condition but the difference was smaller than the group differences seen for the comparison conditions. The current study involves two conditions that each involve spaced retrieval, with only the particular retrieval schedule distinguishing the two. We are hopeful that the expanding condition will show narrower differences between the two groups of children than the standard condition.

6. Conditions and Keywords

Primary Disease or Condition Being Studied in the Trial, or the Focus of the Study
Developmental Language Disorder, Specific Language Impairment, Language Development
Keywords
Word learning, Word retrieval, Preschool-aged children

7. Study Design

Primary Purpose
Basic Science
Study Phase
Not Applicable
Interventional Study Model
Single Group Assignment
Model Description
Within-participant design in which each child learns words under two conditions.
Masking
None (Open Label)
Allocation
N/A
Enrollment
28 (Actual)

8. Arms, Groups, and Interventions

Arm Title
Retrieval practice during word learning: expanding schedule
Arm Type
Experimental
Arm Description
Each child will learn 8 novel nouns referring to unfamiliar plants and animals ("nepp") and a related "meaning" ("a nepp likes rain") . Four nouns will be learned in an expanding spaced retrieval practice condition; four will be learned in a "standard" spaced retrieval practice condition. In the "standard" spaced retrieval practice condition, the number of other words intervening between hearing the target and an attempt to retrieve it will increase from 0 to 3 words. In the expanding spaced retrieval practice condition, the number of other words intervening between hearing the target and an attempt to retrieve it will increase gradually from 0 to 1 to 3 words.
Intervention Type
Behavioral
Intervention Name(s)
Retrieval-based word learning
Intervention Description
Learning sessions: Novel nouns and their meanings are practiced on Day 1. In both conditions, retrieval trials will initially be immediate. In the expanding condition, the next two retrieval trials will each occur after 1 intervening word and then after 3 intervening words for the remainder of the session. In the standard condition, all remaining retrieval trials will occur after 3 intervening words. This practice schedule is repeated the next day (Day 2). Testing sessions: 5 minutes after the learning session on Day 2, the child is asked to recall each word and its meaning twice. One week after Day 1, this recall test is completed again. At this time, the child will complete a recognition test: viewing sets of 4 pictures, the child will hear each novel noun and point to its referent in the array.
Primary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Word form recall accuracy (number of words correctly recalled)
Description
Child is asked to recall and say the word associated with each novel referent.
Time Frame
5 min. after end of learning period
Title
Word form recall accuracy (number of words correctly recalled)
Description
Child is asked to recall and say the word associated with each novel referent.
Time Frame
1 week after end of learning period
Title
Word meaning recall accuracy (number of semantic associations correctly recalled)
Description
Child is asked to recall and say semantic information associated with the novel referent.
Time Frame
5 min. after end of learning period
Title
Word meaning recall accuracy (number of semantic associations correctly recalled)
Description
Child is asked to recall and say semantic information associated with the novel referent.
Time Frame
1 week after end of learning period
Title
Word recognition (number of words accurately identified)
Description
Upon hearing a novel word, the child is asked to indicate (by pointing) which picture among 4 options is referred to.
Time Frame
1 week after end of learning period
Secondary Outcome Measure Information:
Title
Number of correctly identified words on Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 5th Ed.
Description
Upon hearing a word, the child is asked to indicate (by pointing) which picture among 4 options is referred to.
Time Frame
completed at start of study
Title
Years of maternal education
Description
Primary care-giver provides information regarding highest academic degree attained, converted to years of education
Time Frame
information collected at start of study

10. Eligibility

Sex
All
Minimum Age & Unit of Time
48 Months
Maximum Age & Unit of Time
71 Months
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Accepts Healthy Volunteers
Eligibility Criteria
Inclusion Criteria: a significant deficit in language ability (language test score below cutoff for best sensitivity/specificity) or documented age-appropriate language ability. normal hearing no evidence of neurological damage or disease scores on tests of nonverbal intelligence above the intellectual disability range not within Autistic range on Autism screening test native English speaker (can be bilingual) Exclusion Criteria failed hearing screening known neurological damage or disease scores on tests of nonverbal intelligence below the intellectual disability range (standard score less than 75) autism spectrum disorder non-native English speaker
Overall Study Officials:
First Name & Middle Initial & Last Name & Degree
Laurence B. Leonard, PhD
Organizational Affiliation
Purdue University
Official's Role
Principal Investigator
Facility Information:
Facility Name
Purdue University
City
West Lafayette
State/Province
Indiana
ZIP/Postal Code
47907
Country
United States

12. IPD Sharing Statement

Plan to Share IPD
Yes
IPD Sharing Plan Description
Data that contribute to our final published results will be made available at clinicaltrials.gov. Data will include de-identified 5 minute and one-week form and meaning recall scores and one-week recognition scores, as well as children's age in months at time of study participation, raw PPVT scores and an SES measure (years of maternal education).
IPD Sharing Time Frame
Data will become available after the results are published or within 12 months from the primary completion date (i.e., the date of final data collection for the primary outcome measure).
IPD Sharing Access Criteria
De-idientified data will be made publicly available at clinicaltrials.gov

Learn more about this trial

Retrieval-Based Word Learning in Developmental Language Disorder

We'll reach out to this number within 24 hrs